Preferred Citation: Emmons, Louise H. Tupai: A Field Study of Bornean Treeshrews. Berkeley:  University of California Press,  c2000 2000. http://ark.cdlib.org/ark:/13030/kt1k4019fk/


 
Tupai:


1

1. Tupai:

An Introduction

Treeshrews

[1] I use the single word treeshrew, rather than tree shrew or tree-shrew, to distinguish these animals as clearly as possible from genuine shrews of the order Insectivora (longtailed shrew, pygmy shrew, etc.).

suffer from chronic mistaken identity: first, they are not shrews; second, most are not found in trees; and third, what they really are (among mammalian orders) has never been agreed on. The first treeshrew recorded by a Western naturalist was collected in 1780 by William Ellis, surgeon to Captain Cook's expedition (Lyon 1913). He thought it was a squirrel. Indeed, at first glance, treeshrews are so much like squirrels that people who live where they occur often confuse the two, and they are known by the same common name, tupai, in the Malay/Indonesian language. This local name was the basis for the first treeshrew generic name, Tupaia, and its family, Tupaiidae. Forty years passed before taxonomists recognized treeshrews as different from squirrels (Lyon 1913). These animals, which would better have been called squirrelshrews, are in fact like no others.

The best-known treeshrews, in the genus Tupaia, are active, alert creatures that border on the neurotic. They are squirrel-sized, brownish mammals with large, dark, lashless eyes; short, bare ears; and a large, wet nosepad like a pencil eraser on the tip of a long muzzle (fig. 1.1). They have squirrel-like feet; soft, dense fur; inconspicuous whiskers; and a long bushy tail that flicks upward. When alarmed, they chatter, whine, or whistle.


2
figure

Fig. 1.1. A portrait of a Tupaia tana.

At a sudden noise, they flinch as if struck. It is hard to escape their notice, and wary species are difficult to watch for more than a few moments. One usually catches a brief glimpse of a small brown animal flashing across a trail or racing down a log. Treeshrews bound with springing leaps from sapling to sapling, ferret slowly along the ground, or rustle through the foliage of a tree. Close up, there is something alien about treeshrews, with the ancient expression in their wide eyes and their long down-turned mouths.

WHAT IS A TREESHREW?

The question, What is a treeshrew? has been asked many times, almost always while seeking the answer to the treeshrew's place in evolutionary history. In this book I am mainly concerned with other questions, but because a living animal is a result of its history, I begin with a brief overview of thought on treeshrew taxonomy and phylogeny—the search for their true, but often mistaken, identity.

Before 1900 treeshrews were generally thought to be in the order Insectivora and related to the true shrews, but by the first decade of the twentieth century comparative anatomists separated them from that order and proposed a closer relationship to other groups, especially elephant


3
shrews (Macroscelididae; e.g., Gregory 1910). In 1922 Carlsson placed them in the order Primates, and this was subsequently supported with monumental anatomical studies by Le Gros Clark (1925, 1959; see also Elliot 1971). This view dominated for the next half century (e.g., Simpson 1945), stimulating mountains of comparative work on morphology and physiology that was strongly focused on the paradigm of treeshrews as basal primates (although there was never total consensus on their placement in that order). In 1980 a fresh outlook was brought by the application of modern methods of analysis for phylogenetic reconstruction. In a comprehensive, multiauthor review, nearly every wellstudied set of features of treeshrew anatomy was compared to those of possibly related taxa of other orders (Luckett 1980). The editor, W. P. Luckett, concluded, “Following evaluations of the available dental, cranial, postcranial, neuroanatomical, reproductive, developmental, and molecular evidence, the contributors were in general agreement that tupaiids do not appear to share any uniquely derived features with living or fossil primates that would warrant inclusion of tree shrews in a monophyletic order Primates” (1980: viii). Calling treeshrews primates was another case of mistaken identity.

This has returned us to perspectives similar to those of 1910 and earlier. Several hypotheses, one more than a century old, have grouped treeshrews with other mammalian orders, primarily Macroscelidea, Dermoptera (flying lemurs), and Lagomorpha (rabbits), and into a superorder with these and/or primates and bats, but there is still no consensus. Clearly, the last ancestor that tupaiids shared with members of other living orders was so long ago that no unambiguous set of morphological characters allies treeshrews more closely with one order than with another. Along with most other mammalogists, I consider treeshrews to belong in their own order,Scandentia Wagner, 1855, and await more robust evidence of their deeper systematic relationships. Treeshrews are treeshrews.

The only complete review of the genera and species of treeshrews was the classic work of Lyon (1913), who reviewed all available forms, named many new ones, and in the process discovered many important physical features (taxonomic characters) useful for classification. Lyon recognized six genera and forty-six species of treeshrews. Since then, many of these have been grouped together as synonyms, especially island populations of the genus Tupaia, to the point that only five genera and sixteen species were recognized (Corbet and Hill 1991, 1992; Honacki, Kinman, and Koeppl 1982). However,Wilson (1993) recently reversed this trend by


4
recognizing nineteen species. I follow Wilson's classification, which is pertinent to my work because it gives species status to Tupaia longipes, a treeshrew I studied, and distinguishes this Bornean population from the West Malaysian T. glis. This distinction seems to be supported by our data. A modern revision of all treeshrews is overdue, and when this is done a number of other forms now buried in synonymy are likely to be resurrected to species rank. However, the species whose ecology and behavior I describe here are distinct biological entities, and changes in future taxonomies will not invalidate or obscure the subjects of this work. The order Scandentia is endemic to the Indomalayan region and the Philippines, and Borneo, with ten species, is overwhelmingly the center of living treeshrew diversity (Corbet and Hill 1992). To Borneo, then, I went to study them, because only in Borneo could the full range of treeshrews be found together.

WHY STUDY TREESHREWS?

As representatives of one of only twenty-six living orders of mammals, treeshrews are worth studying just to discover the characteristics of a major branch of the Mammalia, but aside from pure curiosity, there are other reasons to find out more about these obscure small animals. Although treeshrews are no longer considered primates, they are thought by some to belong to a group of closely related orders (a grandorder), the Archonta, which besides treeshrews includes the primates, bats, and flying lemurs. In this group treeshrews are the least specialized (most primitive) members. As such, they may still be the most closely related living models of the very earliest primate ancestors of the late Cretaceous period. Their lifestyles can thus provide a window on our earliest antecedents, and perhaps a view of why evolution may have taken the direction it did.

Almost all of the 220 million years of mammalian history are recorded only by fossil bones, more often than not from mere fragments, a teaspoonful of teeth. For the first two-thirds of mammalian history, all mammals were small to tiny carnivores or insectivores (Lillegraven, Kielan-Jaworowska, and Clemens 1979). From these inconspicuous creatures came the great Paleocene evolutionary radiations of mammals, which followed the extinction of the dinosaurs at the end of the Cretaceous period 65 million years ago. We can only guess how these early mammals might have used their habitats. Such guesses are largely built from analogies drawn from living species that have similar morphology: we assume


5
that the saber-toothed marsupial Thylacosmilus was a carnivore like the saber-toothed cats; and that the strange South American litoptern ungulate Macrauchenia had a trunk and browsed on leaves, because its skull has a single nostril between the eyes, as do elephants today (Benton 1991). Morphology can sketch only the faintest outline of how an animal really lives. It may suggest that a species eats insects; but what kind of insects? Where does it find them? Does it feed by day, or night, or both? Does it live alone, have a territory, or share its home with others? Treeshrews have many morphological similarities to early, or primitive, mammals. One of the reasons to study them is to find out what such animals do and what they are capable of. Are different species much alike in how they use their environment? Can we predict the details of their lifestyle from their structure? What, ecologically speaking, is a primitive mammal? Is there such a thing? Can we infer anything about mammalian evolution from the behavior of living species? By studying how treeshrews behave and live in their natural habitats, we can perhaps start to answer such questions.

EARLIER STUDIES

During the half century of treeshrew glory, when treeshrews were considered primates, enormous quantities of research were done on their anatomy and physiology, and also some behavioral work on laboratory colonies. A bibliography of treeshrews published in 1971 contains 1,036 references (Elliot 1971), almost all laboratory or zoo studies of captivebred animals. The subjects of almost all of this work were laboratory stock purportedly of a single species (but perhaps three), Tupaia glis and its related forms belangeri and chinensis, whose origins were usually unknown but who were thought to come from Thailand. These became a mythical entity: “The Treeshrew.”

In a pioneering study at Seewiesen, Germany, where Konrad Lorenz had introduced entirely new approaches to research on animal behavior, Robert D. Martin (1968) conducted the first comprehensive study of treeshrew reproduction and behavior with animals kept under conditions where they behaved normally. This was followed a decade later by the only two detailed field studies of treeshrews before my own, both focused on the Malaysian Tupaia glis. One of these (Langham 1982) was a capture-mark-release study based on trapping only; the other was based on field observation of a population of marked individuals (Kawamichi and Kawamichi 1979, 1982). Both represented significant advances of


6
knowledge about this extensively studied yet little-known species, and details of their findings will be compared to mine in each chapter below. A short study by Dans (1993) on the Palawan treeshrew in the Philippines reported population estimates, activity, and feeding preferences in captivity.

I was originally drawn to study treeshrews not only because I am fond of small mammals and these were poorly known but also because they were reported byMartin (1968) to have one of the most enigmatic parental care systems known among mammals. The mother Tupaia births in a nest remote from her own, and, apart from a visit of a couple of minutes once every other day to suckle her young, she seemingly abandons them. Not only that, she was also reported to lack even the rudiments of the behavioral patterns that ordinary mammal mothers need to care for such helpless altricial infants, such as grooming them or rescuing them from danger (Martin 1968).

This, the ultimate in cursory parenting, was known only from captivity, and there was surprisingly little information about the ecology of wild treeshrews that might help us to understand the natural context of this odd behavior; or, therefore, its function. Intrigued, I went to Borneo to learn how treeshrews of several species lived in the wild, both to see if some feature of their lives might give insight into their “absentee” parenting and to try to describe for the first time in detail the natural history of members of this unique order.

In this study I again asked, What is a treeshrew? but with a different subset of questions. I asked, What is a living treeshrew? How does it act in its environment? What does it do, where does it go, what does it eat, where does it sleep? How does it raise its young? Do different species do these things differently? This book describes what I discovered. I show that “The Treeshrew” does not exist, for each of the six species that I followed has a distinct lifestyle, although all the species share many common features.

I had spent many years in the tropical rainforests of Africa and South America, studying mammals of many kinds. One of my personal goals for this study was to see for myself how the forests of Asia were similar to, or different from, those on the other continents and how communities of mammals resembled, or differed from, each other across the globe. Perhaps from this perspective I could gain insight into how all rainforest communities develop and are maintained.


Tupai:
 

Preferred Citation: Emmons, Louise H. Tupai: A Field Study of Bornean Treeshrews. Berkeley:  University of California Press,  c2000 2000. http://ark.cdlib.org/ark:/13030/kt1k4019fk/